Reciprocal altruism: Wikis

Advertisements
  

Note: Many of our articles have direct quotes from sources you can cite, within the Wikipedia article! This article doesn't yet, but we're working on it! See more info or our list of citable articles.

Encyclopedia

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Reciprocal altruism is a concept, introduced into evolutionary biology by Robert Trivers, which explains the evolution of cooperation as instances of mutually altruistic acts. The concept is close to the one of Tit for Tat known in game theory.

Contents

Theory

The concept of “Reciprocal Altruism”, as introduced by Trivers, suggests that altruism, defined as an act of helping someone else although incurring some cost for this act, could have evolved since it might be beneficial to incur this cost if there is a chance of being in a reverse situation where the person whom I helped before may perform an altruistic act towards me.[1] Putting this into the form of a strategy in a repeated prisoner’s dilemma would mean to cooperate unconditionally in the first period and behave cooperatively (altruistically) as long as the other agent does as well.[1] If chances of meeting another reciprocal altruist are high enough or the game is repeated for a long enough amount of time, this form of altruism can evolve within a population. This is very close to the notion of Tit for Tat introduced by Anatol Rapoport, although there still seems a slight distinction in that Tit for Tat cooperates in the first period and from thereon always replicates an opponent’s previous action, whereas “Reciprocal Altruists” stop cooperation in the first instance of non-cooperation by an opponent and stay non-cooperative from thereon. This distinction leads to the fact that in contrast to reciprocal altruism, Tit for Tat may be able to restore cooperation under certain conditions despite cooperation having broken down.

Stephens shows a set of necessary and jointly sufficient conditions “… for an instance of reciprocal altruism:[2]

  1. the behaviour must reduce a donor's fitness relative to a selfish alternative;
  2. the fitness of the recipient must be elevated relative to non-recipients;
  3. the performance of the behaviour must not depend on the receipt of an immediate benefit;
  4. conditions 1, 2, and 3 must apply to both individuals engaging in reciprocal helping.”

There are two additional conditions necessary “…for reciprocal altruism to evolve:

  1. A mechanism for detecting 'cheaters' must exist.
  2. A large (indefinite) number of opportunities to exchange aid must exist.”

The first two conditions are necessary for altruism as such, while the third is distinguishing reciprocal altruism from simple mutualism and the fourth makes the interaction reciprocal. Condition number five is required as otherwise non-altruists may always exploit altruistic behaviour without any consequences and therefore evolution of reciprocal altruism would not be possible. However, it is pointed out that this “conditioning device” does not need to be conscious. Condition number six is required to avoid cooperation breakdown through backwards induction—a possibility suggested by game theoretical models.[2]

Examples

The following examples could be understood as altruism. However, showing reciprocal altruism in an unambiguous way requires more evidence as will be shown later.

Advertisements

Cleaner fish

The first example is that of the cleaner fish. Aside from the mutuality and the clear advantageous symbiosis of the cleaner and the host, which cannot be interpreted in terms of altruism, the host displays some additional behaviour that meets the criteria for altruism: The host allows the cleaner free entrance and exit and does not eat the cleaner, even after the cleaning is done.[3][4][5][6] The host signals the cleaner it is about to depart the cleaner's locality, even when the cleaner is not in its body. The host may chase off possible dangers to the cleaner. [6]

The following evidence supports the hypothesis:

The cleaning by cleaners is essential for the host. In the absence of cleaners the hosts leave the locality or suffer from injuries done by ecto-parasites. [7] Difficulty and danger in finding a cleaner. Hosts leave their element to get cleaned. [6] Others wait no longer than 30 seconds before searching for cleaners elsewhere. [3]

A key element in the establishment of reciprocal altruism is that the same two individuals interact repeatedly. This is conditioned by site specifics of either individuals (spatial condition) and by a large enough lifespan of the species (temporal condition). Surprisingly there is sufficient evidence that individual cleaners and hosts indeed interact repeatedly.[5] [7] [8]

This example meets the criteria that are described in Robert Trivers’ model. However, some important elements, which are essential to establish reciprocity, are not yet shown: The criterion that an individual doesn’t expect an immediate payment. In the cleaner-host system the benefit for a cleaner is always immediate. The criterion that failing of one individual to act altruistically will cause the other one to avoid future altruistic acts. This will be very hard to show since such failure will mean death of the cleaner.

If Randall’s claim that the host may chase off possible dangers to the cleaner will be proved right, an experiment might be constructed in which reciprocity could be demonstrated.[5]

Warning calls in birds

Warning calls, although exposing a bird and putting it in danger, are frequently given by birds. An explanation in terms of altruistic behaviour is given by Trivers:

It has been shown that predators learn specific localities and specialize individually on prey types and hunting techniques. [9][10][11][12] It is therefore disadvantageous for a bird to have a predator eat a conspecific, because the experienced predator may then be more likely to eat him. Alarming another bird by giving a warning call tends to prevent predators from specializing on the caller’s species and locality. In this way, birds in areas in which warning calls are given will be at a selective advantage relative to birds in areas free from warning calls.

Nevertheless, this presentation lacks important elements of reciprocity. It is very hard to detect cheaters. Also, there is no evidence that a bird refrains from giving calls when another bird is not reciprocating. And there is no evidence that individuals interact repeatedly.

Another explanation for warning calls is that these are not warning calls at all: A bird, once it has detected a bird of prey, calls to signal to the bird of prey that it was detected, and that there is no use trying to attack the calling bird. Two facts support this hypothesis:

  • The call frequencies match the hearing range of the predator bird.
  • Calling birds are less attacked—predator birds attack calling birds less frequently than other birds.

Vampire Bats

A prominent example of Reciprocal Altruism is seen in the vampire bats described by Wilkinson.[13][14] The bats feed each other by regurgitating blood. To qualify for reciprocal altruism, the benefit to the receiver would have to be larger than the cost to the donor. This seems to hold as these bats usually die if they do not find a blood meal two nights in a row. Also, the requirement that individuals who have behaved altruistically in the past are helped by others in the future is confirmed by the data. [13] However, the consistency of the reciprocal behaviour, namely that a previously non-altruistic bat is refused help when it requires it, has not been demonstrated. Therefore, the bats do not seem to qualify yet as an example for reciprocal altruism. However, a closer look at the data shows that - except for a single interaction - all instances of feeding happened between individuals of the same group, who are on average cousins.[13] Thus, it seems much more probable that this example is a case of kin selection than reciprocal altruism.

Known emotional dispositions as a complex regulating system for reciprocal altruism

The human altruistic system is a sensitive and unstable one. [1] Therefore, the tendency to give, to cheat, and the response to other’s acts of giving and cheating must be regulated by a complex psychology in each individual. Individuals differ in the degree of these tendencies and responses. According to Trivers the following emotional dispositions and their evolution can be understood in terms of regulation of altruism.[1]

  • Friendship and emotions of liking and disliking.
  • Moralistic aggression: A protection mechanism from cheaters acts to regulate the advantage of cheaters in selection against the altruists. The moralistic altruist may want to educate or even punish a cheater.
  • Gratitude and sympathy – a fine regulation of altruism can be associated with gratitude and sympathy in terms of cost/benefit and the level in which the beneficiary will reciprocate.
  • Guilt and repetitive altruism – Prevents the cheater from cheating again. The cheater shows his regret in order to save him from paying too dearly for his acts.
  • Subtle cheating. A stable evolutionary equilibrium could include a low percentage of mimics in controversial support of adaptive sociopathy.
  • Trust and suspicion. These are regulators for cheating and subtle cheating.
  • Partnerships. Altruism with the purpose of creating friendships.

Popular culture

This approach to interactions can be seen as a parallel to the "golden rule" approach from Judeo-Christian tradition, where "giving" a benefit, despite possible short-term disadvantages, is balanced by trust in future advantages.

References

  1. ^ a b c d Trivers, R.L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology. 46: 35-57..
  2. ^ a b Stephens, C. (1996). Modeling Reciprocal Altruism. British Journal for the Philosophy of Science. 47(4), 533-551.
  3. ^ a b Eibel-Eibesfeldt, I 1955 Über Symbiosen, Parasitismus und andere besondere zwischenartliche Beziehungen tropischer Meeresfische Z. f. Tierpsychol., 12: 203-219
  4. ^ Heidiger, H. 1968. Putzer-fische im aquarium. Natur und Museum, 98:89-96
  5. ^ a b c Randall, J. E. 1958. A review of the Labrid fish genus Labriodes with descriptions of two new species and notes on ecology. Pacific Science, 12: 327-347.
  6. ^ a b c Randall, J. E. 1962. Fish service stations. Sea Frontiers, 8: 40-47.
  7. ^ a b Feder, H. M. 1996. Cleaning symbioses in the marine environment. In S. M. Henry (ed.), Symbiosis, Vol. 1, p.327-380. Academic Press, N.Y.
  8. ^ Limbaugh, C., H. Pederson, and F. Chase. 1961. Shrimps that clean fishes. Bull. Mar. Sci. Gulf Caribb., 11: 237-257
  9. ^ Murie, A. 1944. The Wolves of Mount McKinley. Fauna of National Parks, Faunal Series # 5; Wash., D.C.
  10. ^ Southern, H. N. 1954. Tawny owls and their prey. Ibis, 96: 384-410
  11. ^ Tinbergen, N. 1968. On war and peace in animals and man. Science, 160: 1411-1418
  12. ^ Owen, D. F. 1963. Similar polymorphismas in an insect and a land snail. Nature, 198: 201-203.
  13. ^ a b c Wilkinson, G. 1984. Reciprocal Food Sharing in the Vampire Bat. Nature, 308
  14. ^ Wilkinson, G. 1988. Reciprocal Altruism in Bats and Other Mammals. Ethology and Sociobiology, 8, pp. 85-100.

See also


Study guide

Up to date as of January 14, 2010
(Redirected to Altruism article)

From Wikiversity

Altruism refers to helping others without thought of cost or reward to one's self.

Examples

What is altruism? Consider the following examples and what they may suggest or illustrate about altruism. Feel free to discuss.

Boy begging in Agra, India

The photographer writes that "this guy broke my heart. I was already homesick for my own little boys, and then he came up and smiled and signed how hungry he was." → What would you do?
Another person replies that "there are highly organised profesional agencies operating in and around Bombay that teach children the art and science of begging. This guy you see may own a mobile phone and in another 20 years he would own at least 10 boys like these to do what he does today...Beggers are directed to beg in wealthier towns for short periods and return back to their hometown after about 6 months of service. A beggar boy like the one you see above earns roughly Rs 500 a day. ($11). Tax free."
 → Now what do you think? When do you help? When do you not help? And why - or why not? read more...

The Bridge Builder

...The builder lifted his old gray head.

"Good friend, in the path I have come," he said,

"There followeth after me today

A youth whose feet must pass this way.

This chasm that has been naught to me

To that fair-haired youth may a pitfall be. "
"He, too, must cross in the twilight dim;
Good friend, I am building the bridge for him.
from The Bridge Builder by Will Allen Dromgoole

Worker Bees and Queens Bees

Eusocial bees are commonly cited as providing classic examples of altruistic animal behaviour. However, this is debatable because the bees' behaviour still appears to favour genetic kin selection (because the Queen's offspring are more genetically related to worker bees than their own offspring) [1].

Kurt Hahn on Rescue and Service

"...The passion of rescue reveals the highest dynamic of the human soul.
...The experience of helping a fellow man in danger, or even of training in a realistic manner to be ready to give this help, tends to change the balance of power in a youth's inner life with the result that compassion can become the master motive.
...I regard it as the foremost task of education to insure the survival of these qualities: an enterprising curiosity, an undefeatable spirit, tenacity in pursuit, readiness for sensible self denial, and above all, compassion."

See also

External links


Advertisements






Got something to say? Make a comment.
Your name
Your email address
Message